Mittwoch, 11. Juni 2014

Are we in power? The functional neuroanatomy of intentional action

In his talk at the LMU Munich in April 2013, Marcel Brass gives an account of the functional neuroanatomy of intentional control of action. Since he found previous neuroanatomical evidence to be contradictory, he developed a new model decomposing intentional action into what, when and whether components (Brass & Haggard, 2008). Aside from his model, he talks about preconscious determinants of intentional action and discusses the role of free will in it.


The WWW model of intentional action

Marcel Brass starts his talk with showing a video of a patient suffering from the alien hand syndrome. The patient describes that someone was pulling at her hair. After some moments, she realized that it was her own left hand doing it. In general, such involuntary movements don’t seem to be random but rather reactionary to the environment. Among others, Assal et al (2007) have shown that involuntary movements include an activation of the contralateral primary motor cortex, while voluntary movements activate a distributed network implicating not only the contralateral right primary motor cortex and premotor cortex but also the left inferior frontal gyrus and especially the supplementary motor area (SMA) as well as the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA).

In order to map intentional control to more specific brain regions, it is necessary to decompose it in its components. The initiation component is responsible for the timing of the action and the decision component specifies which decision is made. Brass also suggests a third veto component specifying whether an action will actually be performed.

A simple method to investigate the initiation component is the Libet task. Participants sit in front of a computer screen and see a clock hand rotating. They are instructed to press a key at any moment within a given time interval. Afterwards they have to report at which position of the clock hand they decided to press the key, not when they actually pressed the key. The results usually show that the decision to press they key precede the actual pressing by about 200 milliseconds. Interestingly, via EEG recordings it was found that a so-called readiness potential (RP) precedes the time at which the participants reported to have made their decision by about 300ms (e.g. Libet et al, 1983). Brass argues that this RP is related to the SMA and pre-SMA regions. Evidence comes from a microstimulation study by Fried et al (1991). After stimulations of the SMA, participants reported that they felt the “urge” to perform a movement or anticipated that a movement was about to occur. If they were stimulated stronger, they actually performed a movement. 

A problem with empirically investigate the selection component is that in order to select from something external stimuli have to be included. The involvement of external stimuli requires a matching on the perceptual and the motor level between the different conditions. To achieve this, Müller et al (2007) designed a new task. It consists of two conditions: externally triggered and intentional. In the first condition, the participants see a cross on the right or left side of the screen and have to press the corresponding arrow key. In the second condition, they freely choose which key to press and a cross appears on the corresponding side of the screen. The fMRI results show that the rostral cingulate zone (RCZ), which is supposed to be activated during conflict resolution, is differently activated in the intentional as compared to the externally triggered stimuli. The preSMA showed equal activity in both conditions. Since conflict only occurs in the internal condition it suggests that the RCZ is involved in decisions between different response alternatives.

Brass discusses it is arguably plausible that there is a third veto component which determines whether the action will be performed or not. Classical inhibition paradigms like the stop-signal or Go/No-Go paradigm rely on external stimuli to stop an ongoing behavior. In order to investigate intentional inhibition of action Brass and Haggard (2007) developed a variant of the Libet task. Participants sit in front of a computer screen and can freely decide when to press a key. Afterwards, they report when they decided to press the key. Up to now, this is the normal Libet situation. However, in about half of the trials, the participants were asked to form the intention to press the key but at the last possible moment inhibit actually pressing the key. Participants could freely choose on which trial they decided to stop the action. Accordingly they had to report when they intended to act. Interestingly, the fMRI results showed activation in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in the veto situation. As Brass points out, this region is different from regions, which are activated in situations with different response alternatives, and it is also different from brain regions usually activated in the stop-signal paradigm. This suggests that the intentional inhibition of action can be dissociated from externally triggered inhibition.


Preconscious determinants of intentional action

As already mentioned above, in the Libet experiment, the readiness potential precedes the awareness judgments by roughly 300ms. Libet concluded that the brain decides our conscious intention unconsciously. Many researchers have criticized this. One reason is that it completely relies on the subjective report of the participants’ awareness, which is not necessarily a good measure for intention. In 2008, Soon et al replicated the Libet paradigm and tried to predict intentional action via means of fMRI analyzing the fMRI data with pattern classification. In their variant of the Libet experiment, participants could decide whether they pressed the left or the right key and also when they pressed it. While they are doing this, they see a stream of letters on the screen. Afterwards, they reported which letter they saw on the screen when they decided to press the key. For the analysis, the researchers trained pattern classifiers with their resulting data set. After the training, the classifier is tested against a different data set. Additionally, they were not interested in correctly classifying the motor response so the classifier had to do the classification before the actual motor event. The results reveal, that the decision can be predicted already eight seconds before the action by activity in the lateral frontopolar cortex and about four seconds before the action in the posterior cingulate cortex both with a 60% correctness rate.

In conclusion, this means that simple intentional decisions can be predicted seconds before the participants become aware of their own decision but the prediction rate is relatively low. Also, the brain areas that predict the decisions are different from regions involved in explicit decisions.


The influence of beliefs in free will on intentional action

Some neuroscientists claim that they solved the free will question: it simply doesn’t exist. Others argue that the problem is a philosophical one and can never be answered empirically. As Brass points out, the really interesting question is not whether free will actually exists or not but whether we believe it exists. Does a belief in the non-existence of free will change our intentional actions? Vohs and Schooler (2008) addressed this question by developing a social experiment. Participants were assigned to three groups. The first had to read pro-free will statements, the second pro-determinism statements and the third neutral statements. Afterwards, they had to solve problems and got paid for each problem they solved. While they were doing this, the experimenter excused himself from the room and trusted the participant with paying himself out on his own and afterwards shredding their answer sheet. The results show that the pro-determinism group got paid significantly more than the free will group, the neutral group and experimenter-scored control groups. 

But will this high-level manipulation also be reflected in low-level intentional action? Rigoni, Satori & Brass (2011) did a study in the Libet paradigm in which participants had to read either no-free will texts or unrelated texts. The fMRI results reveal that a few hundred milliseconds before participants reported awareness of their decision, there is a difference in the readiness potential between the groups. In conclusion, this suggests that high-level belief manipulation can in fact influence basic neurophysiological potentials.

In summary, we are able to decompose intentional action in different components that are neurobiologically implemented in different regions of the medial prefrontal cortex. It is further possible to predict decisions from brain activity even seconds before participants become aware of their decision. Additionally, high-level beliefs about free will can influence low-level intentional action.

Brass’s research begs the question whether the manipulation of high-level beliefs can influence the predictability of intentional action from brain activation. In further studies, it will also be interesting to investigate why disbelief in free will can lead to antisocial behavior (cheating, overpaying) and what its neural correlates are. 


References
Assal, F., Schwartz, S. and Vuilleumier, P. (2007), Moving with or without will: functional neural correlates of alien hand syndrome. Ann Neurol., 62: 301–306. doi: 10.1002/ana.21173

Brass, M., & Haggard, P. (2007). To do or not to do: the neural signature of self-control. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(34), 9141-9145.

Brass, M., & Haggard, P. (2008). The what, when, whether model of intentional action. The Neuroscientist, 14(4), 319-325.

Fried, I., Katz, A., McCarthy, G., Sass, K. J., Williamson, P., Spencer, S. S., & Spencer, D. D. (1991). Functional organization of human supplementary motor cortex studied by electrical stimulation. The Journal of neuroscience, 11(11), 3656-3666.

Libet, Benjamin; Gleason, Curtis A.; Wright, Elwood W.; Pearl, Dennis K. (1983). "Time of Conscious Intention to Act in Relation to Onset of Cerebral Activity (Readiness-Potential) - The Unconscious Initiation of a Freely Voluntary Act". Brain 106: 623–642.

Rigoni, D., Kühn, S., Sartori, G., & Brass, M. (2011). Inducing Disbelief in Free Will Alters Brain Correlates of Preconscious Motor Preparation The Brain Minds Whether We Believe in Free Will or Not. Psychological science, 22(5), 613-618.

Soon, C. S., Brass, M., Heinze, H. J., & Haynes, J. D. (2008). Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature neuroscience, 11(5), 543-545.

Vohs, K. D., & Schooler, J. W. (2008). The Value of Believing in Free Will Encouraging a Belief in Determinism Increases Cheating. Psychological science, 19(1), 49-54.

Donnerstag, 22. Mai 2014

Excessive Video Game Playing Indicates Poor Satisfaction With Life

Last year, I did an informal experimental study about Starcraft 2, a highly complex real time strategy game. Your success in Starcraft heavily relies on multitasking (in it's every-day use). Multitasking in this sense means that you need to keep a complex mental loop for actions which need to be performed in a rapid succession. Professional-level players normally perform up two 300 actions per minute (actions = button presses).
The study had two main goals. First, to investigate the relationship between playing Starcraft and multitasking abilities. Since I used a flash game and used self-report measures, the methodolgy in this part could potentially skew the results.

Second, and this part was not announced, I read a lot of complaints from people who play a lot of Stracraft 2 in forums. I wanted to check whether these qualitative statements had some quantitative foundations. In specific, I investigated whether the amount of playing SC2 is correlated with  general satisfaction with life.

In total, 70 participants took part in the study. For investigation of multitask abilities I used the flash game Multitask from Kongregrate. The participants played 10 times and entered their results manually into a google form in which I continued the study. Life satisfaction was measured using the widely employed Satisfaction with Life Scale by Ed Diener. After that, participants filled out questions about their playing behavior, most importantly how many hours per week the play, and additionally demographical data.

RESULTS
From the initial 70 participants, 20 had to be excluded from the study. Three because of missing data and 17 due to outlier detection (mostly because they reported several multitask results below 20 seconds whichmeans that they didn't really play). That means, that 50 participants were included in the final analysis.

Multitasking
The longest time reported was 262 seconds. The average of all participants was 70 seconds. I didn't find any connection between "hours of SC2 played" and multitasking ability. Linear models also revealed no interaction with league (which is an in-game indication of a player's skill). Either, playing more doesn't improve one's multitasking abilities or it improves multitasking abilities which are not measured by the Multitask game I used or the results are skewed because I relied on manipulable self-report measures and participants did not report the correct results.

[image loading]
(Looks messy, doesn't it?)

I found a trend (p = .081) that master's players (the highest in-game league) lasted on average 20 seconds longer in the multitasking game compared to the other leagues. This is an indication that multitasking abilities such as measured by the Multitask game are really reflected in Starcraft 2 skill. As expected, age significantly correlates with multitask ability (p = .003; r = -.400). Those in their early 20s were best. (see What do the numbers mean? below)

Life Satisfaction
Playing Starcraft correlates negatively with general satisfaction with life (p = .001; r = -.441). The more you play, the less satisfied you are. Don't play so much! To give you an idea, most players in my data set played around 15 hours per week (M = 14.60; SD = 11.9), the minimum was around 1 hour and the maximum was around 40 hours. Also, not surprisingly, the older you get, the less time you invest in SC2 in total (p = .006; r = -.465).

What do the numbers mean?
The p-value is the estimated probability of how likely we found a difference between two groups although there is none. For a p-value of .03 this would mean that if we generated results completely random, in 3% of the cases, we would find this "difference".

The r-value is an estimate of how big the correlation between the two things is we measure. In the example above, we look at the variance in the satisfaction with life between the participants. Now we overlay this variance with the variance from the "hours of Starcraft played" and check how much of the variance in the one data set can be explained by the other. A value of .441 means that 44% of the variance in the satisfaction with life scores can be explained by the variance in the "hours of Starcraft played" data. Given that one's satisfaction with life can potentially influenced by thousands of things (like social life, sleep, achievements, job, etc) such a high correlation is astonishing!


I think that the connection between amount of hours of weekly play and life satisfaction is interesting, I don't believe that playing Starcraft causes one to be less satisfied with one's life. The more probable explanation is that, if you are less satisfied with your life, lack social inclusion or have problems at home, you tend to retract yourself from society, because it's the source of your problems, and approach alone-activities such as playing video games extensively.

Mittwoch, 21. Mai 2014

10 Shitty Life Hacks

  1. If your phone gets wet, place it in a bag of rice. The rice will attract asians who will fix it for you.
  2. Save money on expensive binoculars by just standing closer to the object you want to look at.
  3. Use the metal part of your seat belt to open beers while driving.
  4. When using a urinal, pull your pants all the way down to ensure no one uses the urinal next to you.
  5. Take a dump in the shower. It washes down the drain and saves time and toilet paper.
  6. Get almost anything for free by simply running away.
  7. Shoot yourself with small calibre bullets so you can gain an immunity to larger ones. Helpful in the event of a home invasion!
  8. If you have dry skin, use steel wool to scrub it off easily.
  9. Microwave your spoon to cut right through that rock hard ice cream!
  10. If you're drowning, pretend you're dead. The water will think you're a corpse and you'll float to the top!

Montag, 14. April 2014

Damals, als wir noch nostalgisch waren


Ich erinnere mich noch gut an die alten Zeiten, in denen wir noch wirklich nostalgisch wurden und uns zurück erinnerten an Dinge, die uns bewegten. Wir saßen draußen unter den Sternen, schwelgten in Erinnerungen, Stunden um Stunden. Dachten an all die Dinge die wir liebten, all die guten Zeiten, all den Spaß den wir hatten, dachten an Zeiten, in denen das Eis noch billiger und das Leben einfacher war. Es schleuderte unsere Zeit mit voller Wucht zurück. Nostalgie war doch was feines.
Doch heutzutage ist das anders. Wir werden nicht mehr nostalgisch. Wir denken nicht mehr an alles was mal war. Wir finden ja kaum die Zeit zurückzublicken und daran zu denken, wie früher alles besser war. Nostalgie wie wir sie damals erlebten, gibt es nicht mehr. Das einzig nostalgische wird uns auferzwungen durch das Internet. Durch Seiten, die sich daran laben, alte Witze zu recyclen, uns an alte Lieben zu erinnern und alte Wunden aufzureißen. Damals musste man erstmal wen finden, um sich zusammen zu erinnern. Heute reicht ein Facebook Eintrag „Hey, wisst ihr noch, wie’s damals war?“ In Minuten füllt sich die Seite mit Geschichten der Vergangenheit. Praktisch ist das, ohne Frage. Aber wo ist die Wärme, dieser Sinn gemeinsam erlebter Dinge, der zwei Menschen bindet?
Ich erinnere mich noch gut an die Jahre nach dem Abitur. Wir trafen uns mit ein paar Schulfreunden am See und dachten an die alten Zeiten. In unseren Köpfen drehten sich Gedankenspiele. Was wär gewesen wenn, wo würden wir heut' stehen? Den ganzen Tag verbrachten wir einfach damit, in Erinnerungen zu schwelgen. Das mit der Nostalgie wurde schlimmer, je älter wir wurden. Es ist anders als es war, als wir anfingen nostalgisch zu werden. Die Kinder heutzutage werden nie wissen, wie es ist, wirklich nostalgisch zu sein.

Dienstag, 11. Februar 2014

Your country's history in 20 words or less

This land is your land? This land is my land - USA

Empire. Gone. Tried to conquer with force. Bad. Again.  Really bad. Conquer with money. - Germany

We were once really important but now we make pasta and shoes. – Italy

Glowing in the dark since 1986. – Ukraine

All is fine. No more questions. Eternal President will lead us to victory. – North Korea

Looks like the sea wants to kill us again. – The Netherlands

We love you England, but get out, please. – Canada

Two world wars, one cup – England

Sure we'll take your money. – Switzerland

Sure we'll go for a walk. - Navajo Nation

Criminals placed on an island that is repeatedly trying to kill them. – Australia

Proud back to back World War avoiders. – Sweden

Thanks so much, Sweden. First you fight Novgorod over us, and then you leave us out in the cold. – Finland

Went to war with everyone for centuries, surrender once, are called a country of pussies forever. - France

We are one of the oldest and wisest cultures. Welcome to Dell technical support, my name is Steve. – India

Vodka, monarchs, oppression, space, hockey, new flag, Putin, vodka. – Russia

Muslims in India hated Hindus. Created new country for muslims. No hindus. Hate each other. – Pakistan

Maori come and kill birds. Whites come and kill Maori. Lord of the Rings filmed. – New Zealand

We had a huge empire, now everyone thinks we're Spain. – Portugal

No potatoes. Potatoes. No potatoes. Fine, thanks. – Ireland

No potatoes. – Latvia

Stuff frozen. Stuff slightly less frozen. – Antarctica

We did a bunch of shit for mankind like 10000 years ago, now we're bankrupt. – Greece

Raped, looted and pillaged our way to being the most content people in the world. – Denmark

Viking glory. Ruled by others. Oil. – Norway

Digest with re-edit from here

Sonntag, 3. Februar 2013

5 TEDtalks every becoming scientist should have seen

These talks are not only good because of their content but also because of their exceptional presentation. If you want to learn how to give a presentation, watch these.

Jill Bolte Taylor's stroke of insight
Jill Bolte Taylor got a research opportunity few brain scientists would wish for: She had a massive stroke, and watched as her brain functions -- motion, speech, self-awareness -- shut down one by one. An astonishing story.

Dan Pink: The puzzle of motivation
Career analyst Dan Pink examines the puzzle of motivation, starting with a fact that social scientists know but most managers don't: Traditional rewards aren't always as effective as we think. Listen for illuminating stories -- and maybe, a way forward.

Ben Goldacre: What doctors don't know about the drugs they prescribe
When a new drug gets tested, the results of the trials should be published for the rest of the medical world -- except much of the time, negative or inconclusive findings go unreported, leaving doctors and researchers in the dark. In this impassioned talk, Ben Goldacre explains why these unreported instances of negative data are especially misleading and dangerous.
Vilayanur Ramachandran tells us what brain damage can reveal about the connection between celebral tissue and the mind, using three startling delusions as examples.
Dan Gilbert, author of "Stumbling on Happiness," challenges the idea that we’ll be miserable if we don’t get what we want. Our "psychological immune system" lets us feel truly happy even when things don’t go as planned.

Samstag, 19. Januar 2013

Die Vollstreckung der Todesstrafe


Gestern* sah ich die sehr interessante, zum Nachdenken anregende Dokumentation „How To Kill A Human Being.“ Darin ging es allerdings nicht um eine Anleitung, wie man den Nachbarn am effektivsten und geräuschlosesten um die Ecke bringt, sondern um die Methode des Tötens in staatlichem Auftrag. Todesstrafe ansich ja ist schon ein sehr kontroverses Thema. Derzeit findet sie noch in Teilen der USA, Großteilen Afrikas und im Süden Asiens Anwendung. Man schätzt, dass es seit 1976 allein in den USA bis zu 100 Fehlurteile, Justizirrtümer und Hinrichtungen Unschuldiger gab. Ich möchte hier aber weniger auf das für und wider der Todesstrafe eingehen, sondern auf die Art, wie sie vollstreckt wird. Ich werde dabei Bezug zu der Frage herstellen, was wir uns generell unter humanem Töten vorstellen und ob die besprochene Methode diese Kriterien erfüllt.
Was also ist humanes Töten? Ohne groß darüber nachzudenken würde ich sagen, dass es schnell gehen muss, schmerzfrei und der Körper dabei möglichst unbeschadet bleiben soll. Derzeit gibt es verschiedene Methoden, die aktiv benutzt werden: Die Todesspritze, der elektrische Stuhl, Erhängen und eine Art Gaskammer, wie sie im zweiten Weltkrieg benutzt wurde, sind wohl die populärsten Methoden. In der Dokumentation untersucht der Autor nun die aktuellen Methoden auf ihren humanitären Charakter.

Elektrischer Stuhl: Der Verurteilte soll mit 2400 V über 15 Sekunden so geschockt werden, dass das Herz stehen bleibt. Dazu gibt’s n nassen Naturschwamm auf den Kopf, der eine extrem gute Leitfähigkeit besitzt, um die Zeit zu verkürzen und die Schmerzen zu vermindern. Nur scheinen die Exekutoren oft zu dämlich zu sein und vergessen den Schwamm oder benutzen einen synthetischen, der dann eher dazu führt, dass der Kopf gebraten wird. Alles sehr unschön anzusehen (wurde an nem toten Schwein demonstriert). Und weh tut es bestimmt auch.

Erhängen: Wie viele fälschlicherweise annehmen, soll der Tod nicht durch Ersticken, sondern durch Genickbruch erfolgen. Dazu gibt es bestimmte Tabellen, wie lang das Seil sein muss, damit das Gewicht des eigenen Körpers auch ja den Genickbruch herbeiführt. Hauptproblem: Die Anatomie der Menschen ist sehr unterschiedlich und Körpergewicht nicht der einzige Faktor. Deswegen kommt es bei mehr als einem Drittel der Fälle vor, dass der Verurteilte dann letzendlich doch erstickt, weil das Seil zu kurz war. Dass Ersticken dann mehr oder weniger mit Schmerzen verbunden ist, sollte klar sein.

Gaskammer: Funktioniert recht simpel. Der Verurteilte sitzt in einer Kammer und es wird Kohlenstoffmonoxid hinein geleitet, was die Bindung des Sauerstoffs im Blut verhindert und dadurch zu Hypoxie führt. Dabei kriegt er die Anweisung ruhig und tief zu Atmen, damit das Gas schnell ins Blut transportiert wird und so keine Schmerzen auftreten. Es klingt aber irgendwie paradox den zum Sterben Verurteilten aufzufordern sich aktiv an seiner eigenen Hinrichtung zu beteiligen. In der Dokumentation wurde auch anschaulich gezeigt, dass es in so einer (ähnlichen) Situation allen Intentionen zum Trotz kaum möglich ist, ruhig zu atmen. Wenn man sich nicht an die Anweisungen hält wird es auf jeden Fall sehr schmerzvoll, man fängt an aus allen Körperöffnungen zu bluten und allgemein wird’s sehr unschön.

Todesspritze: Die wohl populärste aktuelle Methode besteht aus einem 3-Schritte-Prozess. Als erstes wird ein narkotisierendes Mittel injiziert, sodass man bewusstlos wird. Als zweites folgt eine Substanz die zur vollständigen Paralyse führt (innere Organe funktionieren noch). Abschließend wird dann noch ein Toxin injiziert, was den vollständigen Kreislaufstillstand zur Folge hat. Allgemein wird diese Methode als schmerzfrei angesehen und da man nach fünf Minuten bewusstlos ist, geht es für den Verurteilten auch recht schnell vorbei. Nun ist es aber so, dass die ersten beiden Schritte auch teilweise in exakt dieser Form in bestimmten Operationen benutzt werden. In letzter Zeit häufen sich Augenzeugenberichte, dass die Narkose nicht lange genug anhält und der Patient kurz nach Einsetzen der Paralyse wieder Bewusstsein erlangt. Betroffene schildern, dass es sich anfühlt, als würde Öl durch die Adern fließen und man würde innerlich qualvoll verbrennen. Abgesehen davon werden die Spritzen von unqualifizierten Technikern gesetzt, da die Ärzte aufgrund ihres hippokratischen Eids nicht dazu gezwungen werden sollen. Dadurch gibt es immer wieder schmerzhafte Komplikationen. Eigentlich unvorstellbar, wie ich finde. Große Sauerei alles.

Keine der aktuellen Methoden scheint also garantiert schmerzfrei, schnell und lässt dann den Raum auch noch einigermaßen sauber. Allerdings existiert eine, die so aber noch nicht angewendet wird. Eine andere Art der Hypoxie, welche mit ihren Gefahren vor allem Extrembergsteigern bekannt sein sollte. Wenn man sich ohne Atemgerät länger in großer Höhe (~3000 Meter) aufhält, erleidet das Gehirn eine Unterversorgung von Sauerstoff, Hypoxie also. Allerdings eine andere Art. Die Sauerstoffversorgung nimmt nicht rapide ab, sondern wird nur langsam immer geringer. Das gefährliche daran ist: Man merkt es nicht. Diese Art der hypoxie verursacht absolut keine Schmerzen. Im Gegenteil, man geht durch die Mangelversorgung langsamin einen Zustand der Euphorie über. Dies wurde auch in einem Experiment nochmal dargestellt. Laut Aussagen des betreuenden Arztes war die Versuchsperson höchstens 10 Sekunden davon entfernt das Bewusstsein zu verlieren, währen sie selbst hinterher aussagte, dass sie dachte das Experiment sei fehlgeschalgen, weil sie alle ihr gestellten Aufgaben richtig beantwortete (was sie nicht tat) und nichts sonst bemerkte. Sie war nicht mal in der Lage sich selbst das Atemgerät wieder selbstständig anzuziehen, da es ihr „irgendwie einfach äußerst unwichtig“ erschien. Diese Form der Hypoxie könne laut Dokumentation auch durch ein bestimmtes Gas hervorgerufen werden, was die sozusagen „perfekte Tötungsmethode“ darstellen würde.

Anhänger der Todesstrafe sind erschreckenderweise aber gar nicht so erfreut über diese Methode. In einem Interview sagte einer sinngemäß: „Ich will nicht, dass die Verurteilten in Euphorie sterben. Das haben sie nicht verdient. Ihre Opfer mussten auch leiden.“ Als ich das gehört habe, bin ich fast aus dem Bett gefallen. Nicht nur der Tod, auch der schmerzhafte Prozess des Sterbens (eine finale Folter, also) soll also die Bestrafung sein. Das Leute so denken finde ich zielich verwerflich. Und wo zieht man die Grenze zum Verbrecher selbst? Man tut so, als wär man die Moral in Person, im Grunde genommen geht es aber nur um Vergeltung. Auge um Auge, Zahn um Zahn. Da kommt es mir so vor, als lebten wir noch im Mittelalter (oder der Türkei), wo Ehrenmorde ja noch Gang und gebe waren. Ist es also tatsächlich human, wenn der Verurteilte auch Schmerzen erleiden muss? Eine fallbezogene Humanität sozusagen? Ich kann dem auf keinen Fall zustimmen. Eigentlich bin ich ja ein sehr toleranter Mensch, aber solch eine Missachtung meiner moralischen Grundsätze kann ich weder nachvollziehen, noch akzeptieren. Für mich war es auf jeden Fall sehr lehrreich zu erfahren, dass es Fälle gibt, wo meine Toleranz für andere Ansichtsweisen tatsächlich eine Grenze hat.

*Dieser Beitrag wurde am 16. Januar 2012 lokal verfasst und fiel mir heute wieder in die Hände.